Cultural Intelligence, Cultural Identity, Autonomy, Self-Efficacy and Second Language Achievement of Iranian EFL learners: A Path Analysis

Document Type : Research Article

Authors

1 English Department, Faulty of Humanities, Bu-Ali Sina University, Hamedan, Iran

2 English Department, Faculty of Humanities, Bu-Ali Sina University, Hamedan, Iran

Abstract

This study was designed to explore the relation pattern of cultural intelligence, cultural identity, autonomy, and self-efficacy and second language achievement of Iranian EFL learners. The participants were 247 undergraduate students. Data were collected nationwide, from different parts of Iran; that is, from Kerman, south of Iran; Hamadan, west of Iran; Mashhad, east of Iran; and Gorgon, north of Iran. The participants' ages ranged from 18 to 26 and they were mostly juniors and seniors. They completed the following questionnaires: Cultural Intelligence Questionnaire (Ang, et al., 2007), Cultural Identity Clarity Scale (Usborne & Taylor, 2010), Learner Autonomy Questionnaire (Cotterall, 1995, 1999) and Self-Efficacy Questionnaire (Pinitrich, Smith, Garcia & Mckeachie, 1991). The participants’ GPA of three general English courses were considered as their second language achievement. Responses from questionnaires were collected through convenience sampling. The design of the study is based on path analysis. After data collection, the hypothesized model was tested by AMOSE program. The results demonstrated that the variables under study could strongly predict EFL learners' L2 achievement. L2 achievement may largely depend on students’ cultural intelligence, cultural identity, autonomy and self-efficacy. Also, it was reported that autonomy was the strongest predictor of EFL learners’ L2 achievement.

Keywords

Main Subjects


Introduction

The importance of L2 learning has always motivated teachers and researchers to utilize different approaches to determine the factors that influence language learning. Recent theorization regarding L2 learning has started to examine the language learning through cognitive, affective, and social factors (De Bot, Lowie, & Verspoor, 2007). Consequently, due to various reasons, learning a second language could be an unpleasant experience that may prevent the desired learning outcome (Liu & Liu, 2015). Therefore, it is necessary to explore the factors that contribute to students’ learning and facilitate their second language learning process.

In the field of L2 learning, learners’ personality traits often restrict their progress, because these factors can be influenced by various temporal and situational factors (Larsen-Freeman, 2015; van Lier, 2004). Based on this fact, considering crucial individual differences that prepare learners to achieve success and achievement in the process of second language learning is a noticeable theme in current language learning studies (Zatorre, 2013). McCrae and Costa (1999) define personality traits as relatively unconditional behavior that testify to individual's potentials in wide domains of functioning. Pervin and John (2001, p. 3) also state, “personality is the part of the field of psychology that considers people in their entirety as individuals and as complex beings”.

In this study, the researchers build on recent research showing the factors (individual differences) that could facilitate or impede the second language learning process. As Dörnyei and Ryan (2015) state, some particular characteristics may affect language achievement. These personality traits may be completely ignored by language teachers. Therefore, their effects need to be studied and explored.  Based on the previous research findings, a hypothetical model was constructed, including L2 achievement as the dependent variable and four factors (i.e. cultural intelligence, cultural identity, autonomy, and self-efficacy) as the independent variables. In this study, the hypothesized model was then tested through structural equation modeling, and a final model of L2 achievement was developed.

 

Background to the Study

Being familiar with the target language culture could significantly facilitate the language learning progress. Therefore, culture is considered as an important concept that Byram (1989, p. 18) referred to it as” hidden curriculum”. Cultural intelligence is defined as individuals’ ability to function properly in various cultures in order to promote perception, communication, and coordination (Adair, Hideg & Spence, 2013). Earley and Ang (2003) also define cultural intelligence as individual’s capability to act appropriately in different cultural settings. Cavanaugh (2007) believes that training can enhance cultural intelligence. Cavanaugh also maintained that more exposure to different cultural training would lead to the enhancement of individuals’ capability.

There are four cultural intelligence (CQ) factors including: a) cognitive CQ, b) metacognitive CQ, c) motivational CQ, and d) behavioral CQ (Earley & Ang, 2003). According to Ang and Van Dyne (2008), cognitive CQ factor is individuals’ knowledge of cultural environment and state that this factor may exert an influence over individual’s beliefs and manners. Metacognitive CQ factor is defined as higher-order mental process to obtain cultural knowledge for better information processing (Ang & Van Dyne, 2008). Motivational CQ refers to individual’s interest and self-confidence in various cultural settings (Livermore, 2011). Ang and Van Dyne (2008) state that despite challenges in different cultures, this factor leads to learning and functioning appropriately and interacting within individuals. Ang, Van Dyne and Koh (2006, p, 101) define behavioral CQ as “the capability to exhibit appropriate verbal and nonverbal actions when interacting with people from different cultures”. As it has been mentioned, behavioral CQ plays an important role in social interaction.

There have been some studies concerning cultural intelligence. For instance, Ang, Van Dyne and Koh (2006) carried out a study to investigate the correlation between personality and cultural intelligence by the Big Five Trait Model (openness to experience, consciousness, extraversion, agreeability, and neuroticism). The participants’ age, gender, and years of interacting with people from various cultures were controlled. The participants were 338 business undergraduate students. The result of hierarchical regression showed that there was a positive correlation between consciousness and meta-cognitive CQ, agreeability and behavioral CQ, but neuroticism negatively correlated with the cognitive, motivational, and behavioral CQ, and openness to experience was the only factor that positively correlated with all of these four factors.

Another study was conducted by Peivandi (2011) in order to explore the correlation between cultural intelligence and writing ability of 159 Iranian adult EFL learners.  In this study, the participants were at upper-intermediate to advanced levels. To this end, the Cultural Intelligence Questionnaire was utilized to measure participants’ cultural intelligence. The result demonstrated that cultural intelligence is a moderator predictor of writing. In addition, it was revealed that cognitive and motivational CQ were the best predictors of writing. The findings also showed that there was a significant positive relationship between cognitive and motivational CQ with writing.

Similarly, Ghonsooli and Shalchy (2013) carried out a study in order to investigate the correlation between cultural intelligence and writing ability. The purpose of the study was to explore the impacts of CQ on L2 learners' writing in terms of fluency, complexity, and accuracy. A sample of 104 subjects participated in their study. The result of confirmatory factor analysis showed that CQ and cognitive CQ are stronger predictors of writing ability and writing fluency.

Moreover, cultural identity is thought to be a crucial factor in language learning. Usborne and Taylor (2010) define it as “the perceived clarity of the actual information or knowledge arising from one’s cultural identity or identities” (p. 885). In fact, cultural identity superficially seems to be similar with Benet-Martinez’s concept of bicultural identity integration (BII). BII is defined as the degree to which ‘‘biculturals perceive their mainstream and ethnic cultural identities as compatible and integrated vs. oppositional and difficult to integrate’’ (Benet-Martı´nez, Leu, Lee, & Morris, 2002, p. 9).

According to Okazaki, David, and Abelmann (2008), a concept of cultural identity rather than being static, ahistorical, and decontextualized is dynamic, it is historically imbedded and contextualized.  It is also said that personal identity is constructed through having a clear cultural identity (Schwartz, Zamboanga, & Weisskirch, 2008). Usborne and Taylor (2010) state that having clear cultural identity is essential for personal identity, self-esteem, and well-being.

According to Taylor (2002), clear cultural identity refers to having a clear definition of the values, ideological positions and experiences, which leads to psychological adjustment and confident beliefs. Unclear cultural identity would not provide the individual with a guiding template for how to act and function in the world; therefore, it could lead to academic under-achievement and lack of self-esteem (Chandler, Lalonde, Sokol, & Hallet, 2003). In general, cultural identity can be regarded as a guiding template to deal with occupational and educational challenges (Taylor, 2002).

For instance, Eleuterio (1997) and Hoelscher (1999) observed that in classrooms where teachers and students have the same culture, the students benefited from higher engagement, motivation and excitement in language learning. Furthermore, in a qualitative study, Altugan (2015) described the correlation between learning and cultural identity. The findings demonstrated that cultural identity plays a significant role in language learning and teachers should consider its role in the process of language learning.

Another important concept in facilitating students’ academic achievement is autonomy. Learners are encouraged to become autonomous by learning how to learn a foreign language because autonomy “provides the primary impetus to initiate learning the second language and later the driving force to sustain the long and often tedious learning process” (Dörnyei, 1998, p.117). Moreover, learner autonomy was found to be associated with a fundamental construct such as responsibility (Bocanegra & Haidl, 1999). Bocanegra and Haidl (1999) believe that learners are responsible for making decisions, overcoming their challenges, and managing their life. Consequently, autonomy is considered as “feeling that one performs an activity due to one's own will or personal desire, with no external pressure” (Zhou, 2016, p. 90).

Extensively, autonomy is also discussed in the self-determination theory. Deci and Ryan (1985) define self-determination as “the quality of human functioning that involves the experience of choice … [and] an internal perceived locus of control” (p. 38). In Deci and Ryan’s definition, this experience of choice refers to autonomy. In this theory, a continuum was outlined regarding learners’ different levels of autonomy. At one end of the continuum is the intrinsic motivation that students willingly perform a task with no expectation; that is, these students have the greatest amount of autonomy to learn. At the other end, students with extrinsic motivation do not enjoy high level of autonomy(Deci & Ryan, 1985). A large amount of research studies indicates that self-determination (i.e., autonomy) plays a key role in promoting learning (Black & Deci, 2000; Zhou, 2016).

It is believed that autonomy has effective consequences on language learning. As it is stated in research studies, it has emotional, cognitive, and behavioral effects in order to promote learners’ learning and self-regulation (Nget et al., 2012; Radel, Pelletier, Baxter, Fournier &Sarrazin, 2014). In several studies, multiple consequences of teacher-student relationships have been found to be due to autonomy support which includes higher level of learning, positive affect, and achievement (Buff, Reusser, Rakoczy & Pauli, 2011; Reeve, 2009).

A number of studies have been conducted on autonomy. Hashemian and Soureshjani (2011) ran a study to explore the correlation among autonomy, motivation, and academic performance. A sample of 60 Persian L2 learners participated in the study. The findings showed that there was a significant positive correlation between autonomy and academic performance, and between motivation and academic performance. Additionally, no significant correlation was found between motivation and autonomy. Also, Negari and Solaymani (2013) explored the correlation among attitudes to autonomous learning, thinking styles, and language learning strategy use of 92 Iranian EFL students. The results of the study indicated a significant association of self-attitude to autonomy with all the subcategories of strategy use. Additionally, a significant relationship between self-attitude to autonomy and most of the subcategories of thinking styles was reported.

Regarding the importance of factors influencing the process of language learning, León, Núñez and Liewe (2015) designed a hypothesized model to examine factors, contributing to math achievement. A sample of 1412 high-school students completed Autonomy, Autonomous Motivation, Effort Regulation, and Deep-Processing questionnaires. Structural equation modeling indicated that math achievement was predicted by autonomy, autonomous motivation, effort regulation except deep-processing.

In another study, a hypothesized model was designed by Zhou (2016) to investigate the effect of autonomy, social anxiety, and collaborative learning orientation on L2 learning. In total, the sample was composed of 303 primary school students. The path analysis revealed that students with social anxiety had weaker sense of autonomy that results in less learning orientation. It was also found that autonomy directly and indirectly influenced language learning.

It is thought that self-efficacy is another important factor in language learning. Self-efficacy has been defined as the individual’s belief regarding their abilities to successfully perform particular academic tasks (Bandura, 1997). Self-efficacy is concerned with cognitive judgments of personal abilities (Zimmerman & Cleary, 2006). In other words, self-efficacy is considered as a fundamental element of social cognitive theory which appears to be a significantly important factor, because it leads to both individual’s motivation and learning (Pajares, 1996, 2006; Schunk, 1995, 2003). According to the social cognitive theory, four factors play important roles in increasing an individual’s self-efficacy; namely, successful experiences, observing others’ success, encouragement, and physiological factors (Bandura, 1977). In a meta-analysis which was carried out by Richardson, Abraham and Bond (2012), self-efficacy was found to be the stronger predictor of academic performance.

Successful experiences refer to the individuals’ personal experience which is considered as an important source of self-efficacy. The second one, observing others’ success, could lead to increase or decrease of self-efficacy in individuals. Encouragement refers to giving verbal encouragement to individuals to do the given task. And, physiological factors indicate the individuals’ psychological states that can influence individuals’
self-efficacy (Bandura, 1977).

Bandura (1986) assumed that self-efficacy has effect on people’s selections, functions, perseverance and elasticity. As it is argued, when individuals feel stronger self-efficacy, their function, perseverance and elasticity are greater; in contrast, individuals with weaker sense of self-efficacy tend to think that tasks appear to be much more difficult than what they really are (Bandura, 1986). Pajares and Urdan (2006) also state it is not possible to explain aspects of human functioning such as self-regulation, learning, and achievement without discussing the role of self-efficacy.

Several studies were also carried out to explore the effect of self-efficacy on learning outcome. In an investigation, Walker, Greene, and Mansell (2006) studied the association of motivation variables (academic identification, intrinsic/ extrinsic motivation, and self-efficacy) with cognitive engagement of 191 college students. To carry out the study, the researchers made a path analysis. As a result, the study showed that cognitive engagement was correlated with self-efficacy, intrinsic motivation, and academic identification. Also, Tilfarlioglu and Cinkara (2009) conducted a study to investigate students’ level of self-efficacy and its contribution to academic success on a sample of 175 students. The results of the study indicated that EFL learners’ level of self-efficacy had a highly significant role in students’ success.

In another study, Giunta et al., (2013) investigated the relationship among personality traits, self-esteem, self-efficacy beliefs and academic achievement on a sample of 426 high school students. The results of structural equation modeling indicated that conscientiousness, openness, and self-esteem were interrelated. The analyses also showed that both traits and self-esteem contributed to the students' perceived academic self-efficacy, which in turn led to higher school grades.

In the Iranian context, Ghorbandordinejad and Afshar (2017) focused on determining the association among self-efficacy, perfectionism, and English language achievement. A sample of 400 Iranian English foreign language learners participated in this study. Two main related questionnaires were distributed among the participants and data were analyzed by means of SPSS version 19. In conclusion, the findings of the study demonstrated that English achievement was positively associated with learners' self-efficacy, and negatively with perfectionism. The findings also showed that perfectionism did not mediate between self-efficacy and achievement.

A model was designed by Ardasheva, Carbonneau, Karin Roo, and Wang (2018) in order to explore the correlation among background characteristics, science anxiety, self-efficacy, and science vocabulary learning of 252 Grade 8 students. The findings of the study revealed that the variables under study had strong correlation with language learning.

As the review of the literature revealed, there was positive association between the studied variables and L2 achievement, but, to the best of the researchers’ knowledge, much remains unknown about the simultaneously structural pattern of correlation among cultural intelligence, cultural identity, autonomy, self-efficacy and L2 achievement. Moreover, the extent L2 achievement is depended on the independent variable (i.e. cultural intelligence, cultural identity, autonomy and self-efficacy) is unknown. Consequently, this gap encouraged the researchers to design a hypothesized model based on review of the literature to explore the structural pattern of correlation among these variables through structural equation modeling.

In general, the purpose of the present study was to examine the correlation among EFL learners’ cultural intelligence, cultural identity, autonomy, self-efficacy, and L2 achievement. In this study, undergraduate students’ GPA of three general English courses were considered as students' second language achievement. Generally, university students should pass three general English courses, namely, two four-credit conversation courses (Conversation 1 and Conversation 2); two four-credit grammar courses (Grammar 1 and Grammar 2); three four-credit reading comprehension courses (Reading comprehension 1, Reading comprehension 2, and Reading comprehension 3). The average of the related courses was collected as L2 achievement. To conduct the study, the following questions were raised to examine the model:

1. Does Iranian EFL learners’ cultural intelligence strongly predict their L2 achievement?

2. Does Iranian EFL learners’ cultural identity strongly predict their L2 achievement?

3. Does Iranian EFL learners’ autonomy strongly predict their L2 achievement?

4. Does Iranian EFL learners’ self-efficacy strongly predict their L2 achievement?

5. What is a valid model of interrelationship among the studied variables in Iranian foreign language learning context?

 

Method

Design of the Study

The main purpose of the present study was to investigate the probable correlation among cultural intelligence, cultural identity, autonomy, self-efficacy and L2 achievement. The design of this study is based on path analysis. Therefore, the hypothesized model was made based on previous research findings, as it is shown in Figure 1.

 

 

Cultural identity

Cultural intelligence

Autonomy

Self-efficacy

L2 achievement

Figure 1. The hypothesized model

Participants

The participants of the study were 247 undergraduate students. In this study, data were collected nationwide, from different parts of Iran; that is from Kerman which is in south of Iran; Hamadan which is in west of Iran; Mashhad which is in east of Iran; and Gorgon which is in north of Iran. Participants were all undergraduate students and their ages ranged from 18 to 26 and mostly juniors and seniors. The selection of participants was through convenience sampling. Gender was not considered as a moderator variable in this study. The demographic information of the participants is provided in Table 1.

Table 1. The profile of participants in the study

City

Frequency

Percent

Hamedan

65

26/31

Gorgan

55

22/26

Kerman

66

26/72

Mashhad

61

24/69

Total

247

100.0

 

Instruments

The participants were requested to complete the following questionnaires.

 

Cultural Intelligence Questionnaire

In this study, cultural intelligence was measured by Cultural Intelligence Scale developed by Ang et al., (2007). The original questionnaire included 20 items: 4 meta-cognitive items CQ (items 1, 2, 3, and 4), 6 cognitive items CQ (items 5, 6, 7, 8, 9, and 10), 5 motivational items CQ (items 11, 12, 13, 14, and 15), and 5 behavioral items CQ (items 16, 17, 18, 19, and 20). A response to each item ranged from strongly disagree (5) to strongly agree (1).  Ghonsooli and Shalchy (2013) pilot tested this questionnaire and reduced it to 18 items, to suit the context of Iran. Due to low factor loadings, question 4 from the meta-cognitive CQ and question 5 from cognitive CQ were omitted. Cronbach’s alpha for the CQ questionnaire is (.87), indicating high reliability.

 

Cultural Identity Clarity Scale

In this study, Cultural Identity Clarity Scale developed by Usborne and Taylor (2010) was used. It is originally based on the Self-Concept Clarity Scale (Campbell et al., 1996). Cultural Identity Clarity Scale is designed to measure clarity at the level of the participant’s cultural group and comprises 8 items. The participants should indicate their agreement with each item by circling the appropriate number (0-10). Cronbach’s alpha for the Cultural Identity Clarity Scale is .86, indicating good reliability.

 

Learner Autonomy Questionnaire

The Learner Autonomy Questionnaire was originally developed by Cotterall (1995, 1999) which includes 40 items. This instrument is based on Likert scale, each section comprises four or five options to select from based on the participants' perspectives. The first section
(1-22) is measured on five-point Likert scale ranging from 1 strongly disagree to 5 strongly agree. The second section (23- 26) is measured on three-point Likert scale ranging from 1 often to 3 never. The third part consists of five questions (27-31) that participants should determine their priorities. The last part (32-40) is measured on five-point Likert scale ranging from 1 always to 5 never. The reliability and internal consistency of the questionnaire is 0.75 and the construct validity was examined by running factor analysis. The questionnaire also has acceptable validity.

 

Self-Efficacy Questionnaire

In this study, self-efficacy was measured by Self-efficacy questionnaire adapted from Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire (MSLQ) originally developed by Pinitrich, Smith, Garcia and Mckeachie (1991). The self-efficacy questionnaire comprises 8 items. A response to each item ranged from 1 not at all true of me to 7 very true of me. The reliability of this questionnaire turned out to be. 93, it also has an acceptable validity (Pinitrich., et al., 1991 & Pinitrich, Smith, Garcia & Mckeachie, 1993).

Procedure

In this study, data were collected nationwide, from different parts of Iran including: Kerman, Hamadan, Mashhad, and Gorgon. The participants were selected based on convenience sampling procedure. The second researcher faced an arduous task of travelling to many universities to distribute the questionnaires. After the distribution of the questionnaires, the procedures for completing the questioners were explained to them.

The students’ average was also collected from the universities and was regarded as their L2 achievement. Undergraduate students’ GPA of three general English courses including: two four-credit conversation courses (conversation 1 and conversation 2); two four-credit grammar courses (grammar 1 and grammar 2); three four-credit reading comprehension courses (reading comprehension 1, reading comprehension 2, and reading comprehension 3) was collected as L2 achievement. After data collection procedure, the hypothesized model was tested by the AMOS 24 statistical package program.

 

Results

Preliminary Analysis

Descriptive Statistics

As seen in Table 2, the non-violation of the assumption of normality of data distribution is reported. Thus, to check the normality of the data distribution, One Sample-Kolmogorov-Smirnov Test was run, whose results are reported in Table 2.

Table 2. The Results of One-Sample Kolmogorov- Smirnov Test Showing the Normal

Distribution of the Data

One-Sample Kolmogorov-Smirnov Test

 

Cultural intelligence

Cultural identity

Autonomy

Self-efficacy

L2 achievement

N

247

247

247

247

247

Normal Parameters a,b

Mean

49.39

34.33

164.15

50.03

23.96

Std. Deviation

18.520

14.248

33.379

21.31

5.804

Kolmogorov-Smirnov Z

1.161

1.237

1.999

1.275

.799

Asymp. Sig. (2-tailed)

.302

.117

.122

.023

.241

 

As shown in Table 2, the descriptive statistics for cultural intelligence, cultural identity, autonomy, self-efficacy, and L2 achievement are summarized. The mean and SD for EFL learners’ cultural intelligence respectively enjoyed 49.39 and 18.52, for cultural identity
M= 34.33 and SD= 14.24, for autonomy 164.15 and 33.37, for self-efficacy 50.03 and 21.31, and for L2 achievement 23.96 and 5.80. Thus, the table shows the score of EFL learners respectively on cultural intelligence (p=.30, p>.05), cultural identity (p=.11, p>.05), autonomy (p=.12, p>.05), self-efficacy (p=.023, p>.05), and L2 achievement (p=.24, p>.05) were normally distributed.

 

Correlation Analyses

In order to check any significant correlation among Iranian EFL learners’ cultural intelligence, cultural identity, autonomy, self-efficacy and their L2 achievement, four Pearson Product-Moment Correlations were run. The result of the Pearson product moment indicated that cultural intelligence correlated positively with Iranian EFL learners’ L2 achievement (r=.26, p<.05). Furthermore, the pattern of correlation between cultural identity and Iranian EFL learners’ L2 achievement were in the expected direction (r=.21, p<.05). Additionally, the pattern of correlation between autonomy and L2 achievement was significant (r=.36, p<.05). Also, the association of self-efficacy with Iranian EFL learners’ L2 achievement was reported to be r=.27, p<.05.

 

Main Analysis

Having examined bivariate associations among cultural intelligence, cultural identity, autonomy, self-efficacy and L2 achievement, the researchers now considered all of the variables in one single analytic model using SEM. For the exploratory analysis part, a series of multiple hierarchical regression analyses were run, the results of which are presented in Table 3.

Table 3. Summary of Regression Results for the personality Characteristics

Factors

Beta

T

Cultural intelligence

.25

4.12

Cultural identity

.19

3.12

Autonomy

.39

5.34

Self-efficacy

.30

4.22

 

As indicated in Table 3, results from the first set of regression analyses support the hypothesized relationships among Iranian EFL learners’ cultural intelligence, cultural identity, autonomy, self-efficacy and L2 achievement. These significant parameters respectively comprised those from cultural intelligence to L2 achievement (B = .025,
T = 4.12), from cultural identity to L2 achievement (B = .19, T= 3.12), from autonomy to L2 achievement (B = .39, T = 5.34), and from self-efficacy to L2 achievement (B = .30,
T = 4.22). In general, L2 achievement was predicted by cultural intelligence, cultural identity, autonomy and self-efficacy. As the results show, autonomy is the most significant determinant of EFL learners L2 achievement in comparison with the other variables understudy and cultural identity is the weaker predictor of L2 achievement.

In assessing the fit of the data in SEM, a range of goodness-of-fit indices were evaluated. the Comparative Fit Index (CFI), the Non-Normed Fit Index (NNFI), the Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA), the x2 test statistic, and an evaluation of parameter estimates were utilized in the present study to assess model fit (Marsh, Hau, & Wen, 2004). The results indicated that Comparative Fit Index (CFI) = 0.91, Normed Fit Index (NFI) = 0.91, Bollen’s Incremental Fit Index (IFI) =0.91, Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) = 0.077, and x2/df = 3.0, showing an acceptable fit for the initial model (Table 4).

The main analyses comprised of Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) and Structural Equation Modeling (SEM). These were run using AMOSE Program. Typically, the researchers tested the hypothesized model to see whether the parameter estimates are in harmony with the present theories and previous research findings and report a final model accordingly (McDonald & Marsh, 1990). The final model is represented in Fig 2.

 

Figure 2. Results of testing hypothesized Model

As seen in figure 2, for the whole model, cultural intelligence, cultural identity, autonomy and self-efficacy were found to be correlated with L2 achievement (r2 = .20,
r2 = .16, r2= .10, and r2= .08, respectively). In this analysis, autonomy was found to be the stronger predictor (r2=.20) of L2 achievement. As Table 3 showed, autonomy was the best predictor (t A= 5.34, p < 0.05) of L2 achievement. Thus, this finding appears to be in harmony with correlation matrixes.

To exhibit a valid model of interrelationship among the studied variables in Iranian foreign language learning context, no significant path was removed. The results of these analyses are represented in Table 4.

Table 4. Modification Process of the Structural Model

Fit index

x2/df

IFI

NFI

CFI

RMSEA

Base Model

3.00

0.91

0.911

0.91

0.077

Revised model

1.16

0.91

0.989

0.99

0.017

Acceptable range

< 3

>.90

 

 

<0.08

 

As seen in Table 4, the final model indicated a very good fit. The modified model and the fit indices generated slightly better model than the first model: the comparative fit index (CFI) = 0.99, Bentler-Bonett Normed Fit Index (NFI) = .0.98, Bollen’ s Incremental Fit Index (IFI) = 0.91, root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) = 0.017, and x2/ df = 1.16. The index of IFI is within the acceptable range (>0.90) which indicates the final model is a better model than the initial model (Hair, Black, Babin & Anderson, 2010). The final standardized model is represented in Figure 3.

 

Figure 3.The Final Standardized Model

Discussion

The researchers in this study set out to investigate the possible predictive power of each independent variable (i.e. cultural intelligence, cultural identity, autonomy, self-efficacy) on the dependent variable (L2 achievement). In terms of the associations between cultural intelligence and L2 achievement, our data indicated that achievement was positively associated with cultural intelligence. It was found that Iranian EFL learners’ who received high scores in cultural intelligence achieve higher success. According to Cavanough (2007), more exposure to different cultural training can contribute to the promotion of individuals’ capability. Ang and Van Dyne (2008) state that cultural intelligence leads to learning and proper functioning and interaction within individuals.  The findings of the study in this regard are in line with those of Ghonsooli and Shalchy (2013) who found a significant correlation between cultural intelligence and writing ability. Additionally, they found that cultural intelligence and cognitive cultural intelligence are stronger predictors of writing ability and writing fluency. Therefore, knowing the ideas, values, and cultural perceptions of other countries can facilitate and accelerate the process of language learning. As a result, being familiar with different cultures can be inspiring and empowering.

Regarding the role of cultural identity in L2 achievement, the findings indicated that Iranian EFL learners’ cultural identity were associated with their L2 achievement. The findings demonstrated that learners who get higher scores in cultural identity achieve higher success. Cushner, McClelland and Safford (1992) discussed some programs such as multicultural educations one type of which is “teaching the culturally different” which shows that cultural identity is crucial in both learning and teaching. Examination of how learners’ cultural identity correlates with their L2 achievement are rare. The findings of this study are in harmonious with Eleuterio (1997) and Hoelscher (1999) who observed that in classrooms where teachers and students have the same culture, the students benefited from higher engagement, motivation and excitement in language learning. Similarly, the results are consistent with Altugan (2015) who described the correlation between learning and cultural identity. Their results demonstrated that cultural identity plays a significant role in language learning and teachers should consider its role in the process of language learning. Therefore, it is necessary for students to know different cultural identities and to be familiar with different cultures, values, perceptions, and ideas. It can be facilitative in learning different languages.

The third goal of the present study was to explore whether L2 achievement was predicted by autonomy. In agreement with previous studies, our results revealed significant links between autonomy and L2 achievement. As the results showed, autonomy is the most significant determinant of EFL learners’ L2 achievement in comparison with the other variables understudy. As expected, students who are autonomous, achieve higher success. It was reported that autonomy has emotional, cognitive, and behavioral impacts and contribute to promote learners’ learning and self-regulation (Nget et al, 2012; Radel et al, 2014). Also, multiple consequences of teacher-student correlations have been found to be attributed to autonomy support which includes higher level of learning, positive affect, and achievement (Buff et al, 2011; Reeve, 2009). The findings of the study are consistent with those Hashemian and Soureshjani (2011) who found a significant positive correlation between autonomy and academic performance, and between motivation and academic performance. Students with autonomy can believe themselves more and organize and implement the necessary steps to succeed.

The last research question examined self-efficacy as the predictor of L2 achievement. As expected, self-efficacy associated with L2 achievement. Thus, it could be assumed that the higher level of Iranian EFL learners’ self-efficacy, the higher their L2 achievement. As it is discussed, when individuals feel stronger self-efficacy, their performance, perseverance and flexibility are much more; in contrast, individuals who feel weaker sense of self-efficacy tend to feel that tasks seem to be much more difficult than what they really are (Bandura, 1986). The results of the study are consistent with the findings of Tilfarlioglu and Cinkara (2009) who found that EFL learners’ level of self-efficacy had a highly significant role in students’ success. Also, the findings are in harmony with those of Ardasheva et al (2018) who found that background characteristics, science anxiety, self-efficacy, and science vocabulary learning associated with language learning. Students with strong notion of self-efficacy often have more in-depth motivation and experience positive thrill and motivation and show more persistence in learning and performance; hence, it can help students learn more.

 

Conclusion and Implications

The present study was designed to investigate correlation pattern of Iranian EFL learners’ cultural intelligence, cultural identity, autonomy, self-efficacy and their L2 achievement. Results from the present study are likely to identify predictors of L2 achievement and demonstrate the relative strength of individual characteristics in achieving success. Therefore, the findings demonstrated that the independent variables could strongly predict EFL learners' L2 achievement. As was shown, L2 achievement may largely depend on students’ cultural intelligence, cultural identity, autonomy and self-efficacy. Also, it was reported that autonomy was the strongest predictor of EFL learners’ L2 achievement. These findings show that it is impossible to ignore the influence of EFL learners’ individual attributes on L2 achievement. As a result, the ignorance of these factors may not lead to success.

As was stated, the present study supported the hypotheses that cultural intelligence and cultural identity have positive influence on L2 achievement. Consequently, textbook designers and teachers should take into consideration the effect of these factors while designing activities and practices to make students aware of different cultures among various countries. Furthermore, the positive influence of autonomy demonstrated its possible constructive and significant role. Teachers should provide educational environment supporting autonomy in order to enhance students’ autonomy in the classroom. Moreover, this study provides information regarding the effect of self-efficacy on L2 achievement. Teachers can play significant roles in increasing students’ efficacy by giving various responsibilities to their students.

In general, the findings promise some principal implications. The researchers expect that the findings of the study could help EFL learners especially in employing autonomy as an important individual characteristic in L2 achievement.  Educators and teachers could address these individual characteristics to guide their students to achieve success so that students feel more comfortable in strengthening these characteristics in themselves. The findings may provide clues for the authorities in delivering curriculum for educational purposes. Textbook designers should also put emphasis on these individual characteristics in book development.

Moreover, these results may contribute to the enhancement of the teachers’ training in schools and universities by taking into consideration individuals’ differences besides academic issues. Therefore, university teachers should consider training in both social and emotional skills (Oriol-Granadoa, Mendoza-Lirab, Covarrubias-Apablazac & Molina-Lópezb, 2017). To sum up, the main implication of this study is how to improve students' learning and achievement by considering the role of the studied variables into teaching, instruction, curriculum and assessment methods that may impede students' L2 achievement.In addition, the present study promises several implications for future researchers. The hypothesized model can be extended by taking into consideration other cognitive and psychological variables including multiple intelligence, emotional intelligence, and motivation that may be correlated with L2 achievement.Also, future research is needed to examine the relation pattern of the studies variables and each subcomponent of L2 achievement.

Adair, W. L., Hideg, I., & Spence, J. R. (2013). The culturally intelligent team: The impact of team cultural intelligence and cultural heterogeneity on team shared values. Journal of Cross- Cultural Psychology, 44(6), 941-962.
Altugan, A. S. (2015). The Relationship between cultural identity and learning. Procedia -Social and Behavioral Sciences, 186, 1159 – 1162.
Ang, S., & Van Dyne, L. (2008). Conceptualization of cultural intelligence: Definition, distinctiveness, and nomological network. In S. Ang & L. Van Dyne (2008). Handbook on cultural intelligence: Theory, measurement and applications (pp. 3-15). NY: M E Sharpe, Inc.
Ang, S., Van Dyne, L. & Koh, C. (2006). Personality Correlates of the Four Factor Model of Cultural Intelligence. Group and Organization Management, 31(1), 100-123.
Ang, S., Van Dyne, L., Koh, C., Ng, K., Templer, K. J., Tay, C., et al. (2007). Cultural intelligence: Its measurement and effects on cultural judgment and decision making, cultural adaptation and task performance. Management and Organization, 3, 335–371.
Ardasheva, Y., Carbonneau, K. J., Roo, A. K., & Wang, Z. (2018). Relationships among prior learning, anxiety, self-efficacy, and science vocabulary learning of middle school students with varied English language proficiency. Learning and Individual Differences, 61, 21-30.
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84(2), 191-215.
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: W. H. Freeman and Company.
Benet-Martínez, V., Leu, J., Lee, F., & Morris, M. W. (2002). Negotiating biculturalism: Cultural frame switching in biculturals with oppositional versus compatible cultural identities. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 33, 492–516.
Black, A. E., & Deci, E. L. (2000). The effects of instructors' autonomy support and students' autonomous motivation on learning organic chemistry: A self-determination theory perspective. Science Education, 84(6), 740-756.
Bocanegra, A. & Haidl, A. W. (1999). Language learner autonomy in practice: Possibilities in a foreign language situation. Revista Alicantina de Estudios Ingleses, 12, 7-17.
Buff, A., Reusser, K., Rakoczy, K., & Pauli, C. (2011). Activating positive affective experiences in the classroom: “nice to have” or something more? Learning and Instruction, 21(3), 452-466.
Byram, M. (1989). Cultural studies in foreign language education. Clevedon, United Kingdom: Multilingual Matters.
Campbell, J. D., Trapnell, P. D., Heine, S. J., Katz, I. M., Lavallee, L. F., & Lehman, D. R. (1996). Self-concept clarity: Measurement, personality correlates, and cultural boundaries. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 141-156.
Cavanaugh, N. L. (2007). Cultural intelligence: Factors and measurement. Unpublished Master’s thesis. Massachusetts Institution of Technology.
Chandler, M. J., Lalonde, C. E., Sokol, B. W., & Hallett, D. (2003). Personal persistence, identity development, and suicide. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 68, 7‐130.
Cotterall, S. (1995). Readiness for autonomy: Investigating learner beliefs. System, 23(2), 196-20.
Cotterall, S. (1999). Key variables in language learning: what do learners believe about them? System, 27(4), 493–513.
Cushner, M., McClelland, A. & Safford, Ph., (1992). Human Diversity in Education: An integrative approach. Teaching Education, 4(1).
De Bot, K., Lowie, W., & Verspoor, M. (2007). A dynamic systems theory approach to second language acquisition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 10(1), 7-21.
Deci, E. H., & Ryan, R.M., (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior. New York: Plenum Press.
Dörnyei, Z., (1998). Motivation in second and foreign language learning. Language Teaching, 31(03),117-135.
Dörnyei, Z. (2005). The psychology of the language learner: Individual differences in second language acquisition. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Dörnyei. Z., & Ryan, S. (2015). The psychology of the language learner revisited. New York: Routledge.
Earley, P. C., & Ang, S., (2003). Cultural intelligence: individual interactions a cross cultures. San Francisco. Stanford University Press.
Eleuterio, S., (1997). Folk culture inspires writing across the curriculum. C.A.R.T.S: Newsletter, 4.
Ghonsooli, B., & Shalchy, S. (2013). Cultural intelligence and writing ability: Delving into fluency, accuracy and complexity. Research on Youth and Language, 7(2), 147-159.
Ghorbandordinejad, F., & Afshar, H. (2017). On the Relationship between Self-efficacy, Perfectionism, and English Achievement among Iranian EFL Learners. Teaching English Language, 11, 103-129.
Giunta, L.D., Alessandri, G., Gerbino, M., Kanacri,P, L., Zuffiano, A., & Caprara, G.V. (2013). The determinants of scholastic achievement: The contribution of personality traits, self-esteem, and academic self-efficacy. Learning and Individual Differences, 27, 102–108.
Hair, J. F., Jr., Black, W. C., Babin, B. J., & Anderson, R. E. (2010). Multivariate data analysis
(7th ed.).
Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Hashemian, M., & Soureshjani, K. H. (2011). The interrelationship of autonomy, motivation, and academic performance of Persian L2 learners in distance education contexts. Theory and Practice in Language Studies, 1(4), 319-326.
Hoelscher, K.J. (1999). Cultural watersheds: Diagramming one’s own experience of culture. Social Studies & the Young Learner, 12(2), 12 14.
Larsen-Freeman, D. (2015). Saying what we mean: Making a case for ‘language acquisition’ to become ‘language development’. Language Teaching, 48, 491–505.
León, J., Núñez, J. L., & Liewe, J. (2015). Self-determination and stem education: Effects of autonomy, motivation, and self-regulated learning on high school math achievement. Learning and Individual Differences, 43,156–163.
Liu, M., & Liu, Z. (2015). An investigation of Chinese university EFL learner's foreign language reading anxiety, reading strategy use and reading comprehension performance. Studies in Second Language Learning and Teaching, 5, 65-85.
Livermore, D. A. (2011). The cultural intelligence difference: Master the one skill you can't do without in today's global economy. New York: AMACOM.
Marsh, H. W., Hau, K. T., & Wen, Z. (2004). In search of golden rules: Comment on hypothesis testing approaches to setting cut-off values for fit indexes and dangers in overgeneralizing. Structural Equation Modeling, 11, 320–341.
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1999). A five-factor theory of personality. In L. Pervin& O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (pp. 139–153) (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press.
McDonald, R. P., & Marsh, H. W. (1990). Choosing a multivariate model: Noncentrality and goodness of fit. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 247–255.
Negari, G. M. & Solaymani, M. (2013). The relationship among autonomy, thinking styles and language learning strategy use in Iranian EFL learners. International Journal of Linguistics, 5(1), 332-347.
Nget, J.Y.Y., Ntoumanis, N., Thogersen-Ntoumani, C., Deci, E. L., Ryan, R. M., Duda, J. L., & Williams, G. C. (2012). Self-determination theory applied to health contexts: A meta-analysis. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(4), 325–340.
Okazaki, S., David, E. J. R., & Abelmann, N. (2008). Colonialism and the psychology of culture. Social and Personality Compass, 2, 90-106.
Oriol-Granadoa, X., Mendoza-Lirab, M., Covarrubias-Apablazac, C. G., & Molina-Lópezb, V. M. (2017). Positive emotions, autonomy support and academic performance of university students: The mediating role of academic engagement and self-efficacy. Revista de Psicodidáctica, 22(1), 45–53.
Pajares, F. (1996). Self-efficacy beliefs in academic settings. Review of Educational Research, 66(4), 543–578.
Pajares, F. (2006). Self-efficacy during childhood and adolescence: Implications for teachers and parents. In F. Pajares & T. Urdan (Eds.), Self-efficacy beliefs of adolescents (pp. 339–367). Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishing.
Pajares, F., & Urdan, T. (2006). Foreword. In F. Pajares & T. Urdan (Eds.), Self-efficacy beliefs of adolescents (pp. ix–xii). Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishing.
Peivandi, A., (2011). The relation between CQ and IQ in writing ability of adult Iranian EFL learners (master’s thesis). Ferdowsi University of Mashhad, Mashhad, Iran.
Pervin, L. A., & John, O. P. (2001). Personality: Theory and research (8th ed.). New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Pintrich, P. R., Smith, D.A., Garcia, T., & McKeachie, W.J. (1991). A manual for the use of the motivated strategies for learning questionnaire (MSLQ). Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan.
Pintrich, P. R., Smith, D. A., Garcia, T., & McKeachie, W. J. (1993). Reliability and Predictive Validity of the Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire (MSLQ). Educational and Psychological Measurement, 53, 801-813.
Radel, R., Pelletier, L.G., Baxter, D., Fournier, M., & Sarrazin, P.G. (2014). The paradoxical effect of controlling context on intrinsic motivation in another activity. Learning and Instruction, 29, 95–102.
Reeve, J. (2009). Why teachers adopt a controlling motivating style toward students and how they can become more autonomy supportive. Educational Psychologist, 44 (3), 159-175.
Richardson, M., Abraham, C., & Bond, R. (2012). Psychological correlates of university students' academic performance: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 138(2), 353-387.
Schunk, D. H. (1995). Self-efficacy and education and instruction. In J. E. Maddux (ed.), Self-efficacy, adaptation and adjustment: Theory, research and application (pp. 281–303). New York: Plenum Press.
Schunk, D. H. (2003). Self-efficacy for reading and writing: Influence of modeling, goal setting and self-evaluation. Reading and Writing Quarterly: Overcoming Learning Difficulties, 19(2), 159–172.
Schwartz, S. J., Zamboanga, B. L., & Weisskirch, R. S. (2008). Broadening the study of the self: Integrating the study of personal identity and cultural identity. Social and Personality Compass, 2, 635-651.
Taylor, D. M., (2002) The quest for identity: From minority groups to Generation Xers. Westport, CT: Praeger Publications.
Tilfarlioglu, F.T., & Cinkara, E. (2009). Self- efficacy in EFL: Differences among proficiency groups and relationship with success. Novitas-Royal, 3(2), 129-142.
Usborne, E., & Taylor, D.M. (2010). The role of cultural identity clarity for self- concept clarity, self-esteem, and subjective well-being. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36(7),
883 – 897.
Van Lier, L. (2004). The ecology and semiotics of language learning: A sociocultural perspective. Boston, MA: Kluwer.
Walker, C. O., T., Greene, B. A., & Mansell, R. A. (2006).  Identification with academics, intrinsic/extrinsic motivation, and self-efficacy as predictors of cognitive engagement. Learning and Individual Differences, 16, 1–12.
Zatorre, R. J. (2013). Predispositions and plasticity in music and speech learning: neural correlates and implications. Science, 342(6158), 585-589.
Zhou, M. (2016). The roles of social anxiety, autonomy, and learning orientation in second language learning: A structural equation modeling analysis. System, 63, 89-100.
Zimmerman, B. J., & Cleary, T. J. (2006). Adolescents’ development of personal agency. In F. Pajares & T. Urdan (Eds.), Self-efficacy beliefs of adolescents (pp. 45–69). Greenwich, Connecticut: Information Age Publishing.